INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2504
www.rsisinternational.org
Mathematics Anxiety and Types and Frequency of Errors in
Algebraic Problem-solving among Grade 12 STEM Students
Rica Mae B. Jastillana; Ronnel A. Pacionela; Darwin C. Salem and Genesis G. Camarista
West Visayas State University-Himamaylan City Campus, Philippines
DOI: https://doi.org/10.51244/IJRSI.2025.120800221
Received: 19 Aug 2025; Accepted: 25 Aug 2025; Published: 24 September 2025
ABSTRACT
This study investigated the relationship between mathematics anxiety and the types and frequency of errors in
algebraic problem-solving among Grade 12 STEM students in a public school in Himamaylan City during
School Year 20252026. Using a quantitative descriptive-correlational design, the researchers assessed
mathematics anxiety levels via a validated Likert-scale questionnaire and identified error patterns through an
eight-item algebraic problem-solving test. Errors were classified as conceptual, procedural, or computational.
Descriptive statistics summarized anxiety levels and error frequencies, while Spearman’s rho determined the
relationship between anxiety and problem-solving performance. The instruments underwent expert validation
and were pilot-tested with non-respondents. The reliability test yielded a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.978, indicating
excellent internal consistency. The study involved 30 students from Grade 12 STEM 1, selected through
cluster sampling. While this provided valuable insights, the small and single-class sample limits
generalizability, which is acknowledged as a study limitation. Results revealed that students generally
exhibited moderate to high mathematics anxiety, with female students showing higher anxiety than males.
Conceptual errors were most frequent (Occasional), followed by procedural errors (Rare) and computational
errors (Never). Students committing conceptual errors reported higher anxiety levels than those with
procedural errors, suggesting that deep conceptual misunderstandings may intensify emotional distress.
Correlation analysis indicated a weak, non-significant negative relationship between mathematics anxiety and
algebraic problem-solving performance = 0.227, p > 0.05), implying that anxiety alone may not strongly
predict performance outcomes. Other factors, such as instructional quality, prior knowledge, and coping
strategies, may moderate this relationship. The study recommends enhancing conceptual instruction,
integrating anxiety-reduction strategies, and providing targeted supportparticularly for female students.
Balanced teaching approaches that foster both conceptual understanding and procedural fluency are
encouraged. Findings contribute to STEM education research by highlighting the nuanced interplay between
affective and cognitive factors in algebra learning and informing interventions aimed at reducing errors and
improving performance.
Keywords: Mathematics Anxiety, Algebraic Problem-Solving, Conceptual Errors, Procedural Errors,
Computational Errors, Error Analysis, STEM Education
INTRODUCTION
In today’s globalized and knowledge-driven society, mathematics stands as a fundamental pillar of 21st-
century education. It is essential not only for individual academic success but also for national development,
particularly in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) fields. Recognizing this, the
Philippine Department of Education (DepEd), in alignment with the Department of Science and Technology
(DOST), has emphasized the need to strengthen math instruction under the K to 12 Basic Education
Curriculum, especially among students in the STEM strand. DOST’s push for scientific and technological
advancement requires a workforce well-versed in analytical and quantitative reasoningcompetencies that are
rooted in mathematics proficiency.
At the global level, this initiative supports United Nations (2015) Sustainable Development Goal (SDG) 4:
Quality Education, which calls for inclusive and equitable quality education and the promotion of lifelong
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2505
www.rsisinternational.org
learning opportunities for all. Improving mathematics outcomes, particularly among senior high school
students, is crucial to achieving this goal and preparing learners to contribute meaningfully to a knowledge-
based economy.
However, persistent challenges remain. One such challenge is mathematics anxiety, which research
consistently links to poorer mathematical performance through mechanisms such as avoidance and cognitive
interference (Zhang, Zhao, & Kong, 2019). This form of anxiety is especially prevalent during algebraic
problem-solving, which often demands abstract reasoning, manipulation of variables, and multi-step logical
thinking. When students experience high levels of anxiety, their cognitive resources are compromised, leading
to increased errors and reduced performance (Ashcraft & Faust, 1994).
Errors in mathematics problem-solving are commonly classified into three types: conceptual errors
(misunderstanding principles or definitions), procedural errors (incorrect execution of steps), and
computational errors (mistakes in basic arithmetic). While studies have explored the nature of math anxiety
and its effect on general performance, limited research has investigated the specific link between the types and
frequency of errors committed and students' anxiety levelsparticularly in the context of senior high school
STEM learners in the Philippines.
The interplay of math anxiety with other variables and constructs was explored by Camarista (2015), whose
study is particularly relevant. Camarista investigated the relationship among creativity, self-efficacy, and
anxiety in relation to mathematical problem-solving performance among mathematically gifted Grade 6 pupils.
His findings highlighted the significant mediating role of anxiety, which negatively influenced students’
mathematical creativity and self-efficacy, ultimately affecting their performance. This supports the notion that
affective factors like anxiety can act as a barrier to optimal cognitive functioning, even among high-ability
learners. By considering both emotional and cognitive dimensions, Camarista’s study underscores the
importance of addressing anxiety to enhance students’ mathematical performancea perspective extended in
the present study to include specific algebraic error patterns.
Moreover, 21st-century educational thrusts highlight the development of critical thinking, problem-solving,
and resilience, especially in high-pressure academic domains like mathematics. Understanding how affective
factors like anxiety influence students’ approach to problem-solvingand the kinds of errors they makecan
inform more effective teaching strategies that address both emotional and cognitive learning needs.
This study seeks to address these gaps by analyzing the relationship between mathematics anxiety and the
types and frequency of errors in algebraic problem-solving among Grade 12 STEM students. By identifying
which errors are most commonly associated with high levels of anxiety, educators can better design
responsive, differentiated instruction and intervention programs. The findings of this study can also inform
policy-making and curriculum enhancement initiatives led by agencies such as DOST-SEI, CHED, and
DepEd, particularly in improving STEM education quality in line with national and global development goals.
Research Questions
This study aims to explore the relationship between math anxiety and algebraic problem-solving errors among
high school students by analyzing how different levels of math anxiety influence the types and frequencies of
errors they make.
Specifically, this study aimed to determine the:
What is the level of mathematics anxiety among Grade 12 STEM students towards algebraic problem-solving
as an entire group and when categorized in terms of sex, and dominant type of error?
How frequent do Grade 12 STEM students commit each type of errors in algebraic problem solving?
Is there a significant relationship between the level mathematics anxiety and the Algebraic Problem-Solving
Performance among Grade 12 STEM Students?
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2506
www.rsisinternational.org
Theoretical Framework
This study is anchored on two major theoretical underpinnings: Cognitive Load Theory (Sweller, Ayres, &
Kalyuga, 1988) and Ashcraft’s Disruption Theory of Mathematics Anxiety (Ashcraft & Faust, 1994).
According to Cognitive Load Theory, learners have a limited working memory capacity, which can be
overwhelmed when tasks require intense cognitive processing. Algebraic problem-solving, especially in the
STEM strand, demands abstract reasoning and multi-step operations that can impose a high cognitive load.
When students experience mathematics anxiety, this load is further increased as anxiety consumes part of the
working memory that would otherwise be allocated for solving the problem. This often results in diminished
problem-solving performance and an increase in errorsparticularly conceptual and procedural ones.
Ashcraft and Faust (1994) support this notion through their cognitive interference framework, which suggests
that math anxiety disrupts the brain’s ability to process mathematical tasks by reducing working memory
efficiency. Anxiety acts as a cognitive distraction, impairing both the storage and processing functions of
working memory. This disruption leads students to commit more errors, particularly in problems that require
sustained attention, multi-step logic, and abstract reasoning. Their findings highlight the powerful role of
affective factors in undermining cognitive performance during math tasks. Furthermore, Bandura’s Social
Cognitive Theory (1997) emphasizes the role of self-efficacy and emotional regulation in performance.
Students who believe they are incapable of handling algebraic problems may experience increased anxiety and
consequently perform poorly, which creates a negative feedback loop of low confidence, high anxiety, and
more frequent errors.
Collectively, these theories explain how mathematics anxiety not only influences students’ emotional states but
also directly impacts the frequency and type of errors they commit. Conceptual errors may occur more
frequently among students with high anxiety due to the lack of deep understanding and difficulty in abstract
reasoning, while procedural or computational errors may arise from disrupted cognitive processes or
inattentiveness. This theoretical grounding provides a comprehensive lens for understanding the complex
interplay between affective and cognitive factors in algebraic problem-solving among senior high school
students.
METHODOLOGY
Research Design
This study utilized a quantitative descriptive-correlational research design to examine the relationship between
mathematics anxiety and algebraic problem-solving errors among Grade 12 STEM students. Quantitative
Research encompasses a range of methods concerned with the systematic investigation of social phenomena,
using statistical or numerical data. Therefore, quantitative research involves measurement and assumes that the
phenomena under study can be measured (Watson, R. 2015). The descriptive component of the study assessed
students’ levels of mathematics anxiety and identified the types and frequencies of algebraic errors they
committed, providing a clear picture of their problem-solving difficulties. The correlational component
investigated the natural association between mathematics anxiety and students’ problem-solving performance
without manipulating any variables, aiming to determine whether higher anxiety levels were linked to
increased or specific types of algebraic errors.
Participants of the Study
The respondents of this study were the Grade 12 - STEM 1 students from one of the public schools in
Himamaylan City during the School Year 20252026. The selection of respondents followed a cluster
sampling technique, in which an entire pre-existing group (STEM 1) was chosen as a representative sample for
the study.
Cluster sampling is a probability sampling method wherein the population is divided into groups or clusters,”
and a whole cluster is selected either randomly or purposively for data collection (Alvi, 2016). This method is
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2507
www.rsisinternational.org
commonly used in educational research due to its cost-effectiveness, time efficiency, and practicality in
studying naturally occurring groups, such as a class of students (Saunders, Lewis, & Thornhill, 2019).
The total Grade 12 STEM population in the school consisted of three intact classes. STEM 1 was selected
randomly through a cluster draw, making its 30 enrolled students the study’s respondents. While the sample
size provided meaningful insights, the relatively small number and single-class sample limit the
generalizability of the findings. This limitation is acknowledged in the conclusion and recommendations.
Data Gathering Procedure
The data collection process followed a systematic approach to ensure validity, reliability, and accuracy.
Initially, the research instruments were developed based on relevant learning competencies, with a Table of
Specifications (TOS) created to ensure content alignment. These instruments underwent expert validation,
during which Mathematics educators reviewed them for clarity, relevance, and appropriateness. Their feedback
guided necessary revisions to improve the instruments’ effectiveness.
Following validation, a pilot test was conducted with a group of non-respondents to assess the instruments'
clarity and reliability. Statistical measures, such as Cronbach’s Alpha for internal consistency and item
analysis for difficulty and discrimination indices, were applied. Based on the pilot test results, modifications
were made to enhance the accuracy and quality of the instruments.
Once finalized, the researchers sought formal approval from the School Principal, Senior High School
Department Head, and Class Adviser to conduct the study. Permission letters were submitted to secure proper
authorization. Prior to the administration of the instruments, the respondents and their parents or guardians
were informed about the study’s purpose, the voluntary nature of participation, and the confidentiality of their
responses. Written consent was obtained to ensure ethical compliance.
The administration of the research instruments took place in a controlled classroom setting to minimize
external influences. Clear instructions were given, and students were allotted sufficient time to complete the
assessments. After data collection, all responses were checked for completeness and consistency. Incomplete
or invalid entries were excluded from further analysis.
Finally, the gathered data were encoded and analyzed using statistical software. Descriptive statistics were
used to summarize the findings, while correlational analysis was conducted to determine the relationship
between mathematics anxiety and algebraic problem-solving errors. The results were reviewed by the research
adviser and a statistician to ensure accuracy and proper interpretation.
Research Instrument
The research instrument used in this study consisted of two main parts: a Likert scale questionnaire to assess
students’ mathematics anxiety levels and a set of algebraic problem-solving questions to identify and
categorize errors and evaluate problem-solving performance.
The first part was a validated researcher-made Likert scale designed to measure students’ levels of
mathematics anxiety specifically in algebra. It included statements related to students' feelings and reactions
toward algebra and algebraic problem-solving, rated on a five-point scale: Strongly Agree (5), Agree (4),
Neutral (3), Disagree (2), and Strongly Disagree (1). This section provided a quantifiable measure of anxiety
that could be analyzed in relation to performance.
The second part consisted of eight (8) algebraic problem-solving questions that required students to show
complete solutions. The students' responses were analyzed to identify the types and frequencies of errors.
Based on the frameworks of Hudson and Miller (2006) and Ginsburg (1987, as cited in the University of
Kansas, n.d.), the errors were categorized into three types: conceptual errors, which involved incorrect
understanding of algebraic principles (e.g., operations on variables or properties like the distributive law);
procedural errors, which resulted from incorrect application or sequencing of steps; and computational errors,
which involved basic arithmetic mistakes such as incorrect operations or decimal misplacement.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2508
www.rsisinternational.org
To ensure the content validity of the test items, the researchers developed a Table of Specification (TOS)
aligning each problem with the General Mathematics Learning Competencies. The items were based on the
following competencies: (1) operations on functions, (2) solving rational equations and inequalities, (3) inverse
of one-to-one functions, and (4) solving exponential equations and inequalities.
To assess the quality and accuracy of the students' responses, the researchers used a standardized scoring
rubric based on Polya’s problem-solving strategy.
In addition, the instruments underwent expert validation by mathematics educators to confirm clarity,
appropriateness, and alignment with the intended learning competencies. A pilot test was then conducted with
non-respondents. Statistical reliability analysis using Cronbach’s alpha yielded 0.978, indicating excellent
internal consistency. Item analysis was also performed, examining difficulty and discrimination indices to
further improve the quality of the test items. Necessary revisions were made prior to the final administration.
Data Analysis Method
The collected data were analyzed using appropriate descriptive and inferential statistical tools to ensure
accurate interpretation of the findings. Descriptive statistics, including mean, standard deviation, frequency,
and percentage, were used to determine the overall level of mathematics anxiety among Grade 12 STEM
students and to summarize the types and frequencies of errorsconceptual, procedural, and computational
committed in algebraic problem-solving.
To examine the relationship between students’ mathematics anxiety levels and their problem-solving
performance, Spearman’s rank-order correlation coefficient (Spearman’s rho) was computed. This non-
parametric test was appropriate because mathematics anxiety was measured on an ordinal scale using a Likert-
type questionnaire.
To assess significant differences in mathematics anxiety levels based on sex and other categorical variables,
the Mann-Whitney U test was used for two-group comparisons (e.g., male vs. female), while the Kruskal-
Wallis H test was applied for comparisons involving more than two groups (e.g., performance levels or error-
type categories). These non-parametric tests were selected due to the ordinal nature of the anxiety data and the
small sample size.
All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences) and Microsoft
Excel to ensure the accuracy and reliability of the data interpretation.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Table 1 Level of Mathematics Anxiety of Grade 12 STEM Students towards Algebraic Problem Solving as an
Entire Group and When Categorized as to Sex and Type of Error
Variable
Category
n
Mean
Description
Sex
Male
9
3.44
Moderate
Female
21
3.78
High
Type of Error
Conceptu
al
25
3.82
High
Procedura
l
5
2.95
Moderate
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2509
www.rsisinternational.org
Total
30
3.67
High
Note: 4.51-5.00 Very High; 3.51-4.50 High; 2.51-3.50 Moderate; 1.51-2.50 Low;
1.00-1.50 Very Low
The study aimed to determine the level of mathematics anxiety experienced by Grade 12 STEM students
toward algebraic problem solving, both as an entire group and when categorized by sex and type of error. The
findings indicate that, on average, students exhibit a moderate to high level of anxiety.
As an entire group, students demonstrated a high level of mathematics anxiety, particularly in algebraic
problem solvinga common area where students often experience conceptual and procedural difficulties. This
moderate to high anxiety could negatively influence students’ confidence, problem-solving ability, and overall
academic performance in mathematics (Xie, 2024).
When analyzed by sex, the data revealed that, male students had a mean score of 3.44 (SD = 0.85), categorized
as moderate anxiety. On the other hand, female students had a higher mean score of 3.78 (SD = 0.62), falling
within the high anxiety category.
These findings are consistent with previous research, as a recent meta-analysis confirmed that gender
differences in math anxiety remain robust across settings, with females consistently scoring higher (Xie, 2024).
Similarly, a study conducted in Ghana reported that while both genders generally exhibited moderate levels of
mathematics anxiety, female students’ achievement was more negatively impacted, underscoring the persistent
link between anxiety and performance (Asomah et al., 2025).
While the current discussion focuses on sex-based comparisons, categorizing anxiety levels by the type of
errorconceptual, procedural, carelessmay further illuminate how particular kinds of mathematical
mistakes trigger different emotional responses. Research on algebraic error types supports this distinction,
identifying conceptual errors (stemming from misunderstanding principles) versus procedural errors (slips in
method) as meaningful error categories (Vicinanza, 2024; Yudhanegara et al., 2023).
The data revealed notable differences in anxiety levels based on the type of error committed. Students who
committed conceptual errors exhibited a high level of mathematics anxiety, with a mean score of 3.82 (SD =
0.65). In contrast, those who struggled primarily with procedural errors showed a moderate level of anxiety,
with a mean score of 2.95 (SD = 0.53). This difference suggests that conceptual misunderstandings evoke
more anxiety in students than procedural lapses.
Conceptual errors refer to misunderstandings of fundamental mathematical principles, such as the properties of
algebraic expressions, equations, or functions. Students making these errors likely lack a clear mental model of
algebraic concepts, leading to uncertainty, confusion, and frustration, which may increase anxiety.
This finding aligns with earlier study showing that weak conceptual understanding is a strong predictor of
mathematics anxiety. El Houari et al. (2020) confirmed that strengthening conceptual understanding is more
effective in reducing math anxiety than focusing solely on procedural fluency.
By contrast, procedural errors occur when students understand the concept but misapply a rule, perform steps
incorrectly, or make arithmetic slips. The lower anxiety levels observed among these students may reflect a
perception that such mistakes are more easily corrected and less indicative of deep misunderstanding. Dowker,
Sarkar, and Looi (2016) noted that procedural errors elicit less emotional strain because students retain
confidence in their basic understanding, while Vicinanza (2024) added that the correctable nature of these
errors reduces their impact on anxiety. Taken together, these findings suggest that the type of error students
commit has distinct implications for their experience of mathematics anxiety, with conceptual
misunderstandings posing the greater emotional burden.
Table 2 Frequency of Each Type of Error Grade 12 STEM Students Commit in Algebraic Problem Solving
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2510
www.rsisinternational.org
Type of Error
n
Mean
SD
Description
Conceptual
30
4.71
1.59
Occasional
Procedural
30
3.10
1.68
Rare
Computational
30
1.19
.86
Never
Note: 6.41-8.00 Very Frequent; 4.81-6.40 Frequent; 3.21-4.80 Occasional;
1.61-3.20 Rare; 1.00-1.60 Never
The analysis of students' responses to the eight-item algebraic problem-solving test revealed varying
frequencies of error typesconceptual, procedural, and computationalamong Grade 12 STEM students.
Conceptual Errors: Mean = 4.71, SD = 1.59 (Occasional)
The most frequently committed errors were conceptual in nature. With a mean of 4.71, categorized as
Occasional, it suggests that students made an average of nearly five conceptual errors across the eight
problems. Conceptual errors typically stem from misunderstandings of the underlying principles or
relationships in algebrasuch as failing to grasp variable manipulation, the distributive property, or the
meaning of equations and expressions.
This result aligns with both recent and earlier findings of Kenney (2024) confirmed that conceptual
misunderstandings remain the most prevalent type of algebraic error, while Delastri and Lolang (2023) found
that students who depend primarily on rote rules are less able to transfer or adapt their knowledge to novel
problem-solving contexts.
Procedural Errors: Mean = 3.10, SD = 1.68 (Rare)
Procedural errors were less frequent, with a mean of 3.10, interpreted as Rare. These errors involve incorrect
steps in problem-solving, such as misapplying rules or incorrectly following algorithms. The relatively low
occurrence may indicate that students have acquired procedural fluencypossibly through repetition and
practicebut may not fully understand when and why specific procedures apply.
Rittle-Johnson and Schneider (2015) emphasize the distinction between procedural and conceptual knowledge,
noting that students can often execute steps correctly without understanding their purpose. This study’s result
supports this, showing that while students may follow correct procedures, they may falter when required to
apply deeper reasoning.
Computational Errors: Mean = 1.19, SD = 0.86 (Never)
Computational errors were the least common, with a mean of 1.19, falling within the Never category. These
errors involve basic arithmetic operations such as addition, subtraction, multiplication, or division mistakes.
The rarity of such errors suggests that students have achieved automaticity in arithmetic operations, enabling
them to perform calculations accurately.
This finding is consistent with Dowker et al. (2016), who observed that computational proficiency tends to
stabilize earlier in education, especially for students in advanced tracks like STEM. As students mature
academically, computational skills become more reliable, allowing cognitive resources to shift toward higher-
level problem-solving.
Table 3 Significant Relationship Between the Level Mathematics Anxiety and the Algebraic Problem-Solving
Performance among Grade 12 STEM Students
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2511
www.rsisinternational.org
Math Anxiety
Problem-Solving
Performance
Math Anxiety
Spearman’s rho
1.000
-.227
p-value
-
.227
n
30
30
Problem-Solving Performance
Spearman’s rho
-.227
1.000
p-value
.227
-
n
30
30
Note: p < .01 Significant at .01 level
Spearman’s rho: ±0.1 to ±0.3 – Small/Weak, ±0.3 to ±0.5 Medium/Moderate,
±0.5 to ±1.0 Large/Strong
The results indicate a non-significant negative correlation between the level of mathematics anxiety and
algebraic problem-solving performance among Grade 12 STEM students, as reflected by Spearman’s rho of -
0.227 and a p-value of 0.227 (p > 0.05). This suggests that although there is a weak inverse relationship
whereby higher mathematics anxiety tends to associate with lower problem-solving performancethe
relationship is not statistically significant in this sample.
This pattern aligns with recent studies reporting complex and occasionally non-significant associations
between math anxiety and achievement, particularly among secondary students. For instance, a study involving
primary and secondary students found only weak negative correlations, sometimes non-significant depending
on the grade level and context (Mitchell, 2022). Similarly, research in the Turks and Caicos Islands observed a
small, non-significant negative correlation between mathematics anxiety and achievement among secondary
students (Anderson-Waugh & George, 2024).
These findings indicate that students may have adopted effective coping mechanismssuch as cognitive
reframing or anxiety regulation strategiesthat reduce the detrimental effects of mathematics anxiety on
performance. Furthermore, variables such as instructional quality, intrinsic motivation, prior mathematical
knowledge, and test-taking experience may exert a more substantial influence on students’ algebraic problem-
solving outcomes than anxiety alone.
Taken together, while mathematics anxiety remains a significant affective construct in educational research, its
direct impact on academic performance appears to be complex and context-dependent. Hence, it should be
examined in conjunction with cognitive and contextual factors to provide a more comprehensive understanding
of its role in mathematics learning (Mitchell & George, 2022).
CONCLUSIONS
The study found that Grade 12 STEM students generally experience moderate to high levels of math anxiety,
especially in algebraic problem solvinglikely due to its abstract and complex nature. Female students
showed higher anxiety than males, probably influenced by social and psychological factors like stereotypes
and self-doubt. Students who made conceptual errors reported greater anxiety than those with procedural
errors, perhaps because deeper misunderstandings cause more confusion and stress. In contrast, procedural
mistakes may feel more manageable, resulting in lower anxiety levels.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2512
www.rsisinternational.org
The findings suggest that students most frequently committed conceptual errors, followed by procedural, with
computational errors being the least common. This likely reflects a solid foundation in basic arithmetic but a
lack of deep understanding of algebraic concepts. Perhaps instruction has focused more on procedural steps
than on the underlying "why," making abstract reasoning more challenging. These results highlight the need
for balanced teaching that builds both conceptual understanding and procedural fluency.
The study found a weak, non-significant negative relationship between mathematics anxiety and algebraic
problem-solving performance among Grade 12 STEM students. This suggests that while higher anxiety may
slightly relate to lower performance, the link is not strong or consistent. Perhaps students have developed
coping strategies or other factorslike motivation, teaching quality, or prior knowledgeplay a more
influential role in their math success. This aligns with previous research highlighting the complex nature of the
anxiety-performance connection.
RECOMMENDATIONS
Enhance Conceptual Instruction in Algebra: Teachers should focus more on building deep conceptual
understanding rather than relying heavily on procedural instruction. Using visual models, real-life applications,
and inquiry-based approaches may help students grasp the "why" behind algebraic processes and reduce
conceptual errors.
Integrate Anxiety-Reducing Strategies in Math Lessons: Incorporating mindfulness activities, growth mindset
practices, and low-stakes assessments can help manage students’ anxiety. Encouraging a supportive classroom
climate where mistakes are viewed as learning opportunities may especially benefit students with high math
anxiety.
Provide Targeted Support for Female Students: Since female students tend to report higher levels of anxiety,
schools may consider gender-responsive strategies such as mentorship programs, positive role models in
STEM, and confidence-building activities to address stereotype threats and boost self-efficacy.
Balance Procedural Fluency with Conceptual Depth: While students may be able to follow procedures, they
may not fully understand their application. Teachers should ensure a balance by designing activities that
require explanation, justification, and exploration of multiple solution paths.
Offer Remedial and Enrichment Programs: Establish programs that focus on conceptual gaps and provide
differentiated instruction to meet varying student needsparticularly those who struggle with abstract
reasoning in algebra.
Train Teachers in Math Anxiety Awareness: Professional development should include training on identifying
signs of math anxiety and implementing instructional practices that reduce student stress, especially in topics
known to trigger anxiety like algebra.
Conduct Further Research on Other Influencing Factors: Given that mathematics anxiety was only weakly
associated with algebraic problem-solving performance in this study, future research should investigate other
potential variablessuch as motivation, prior achievement, teaching methods, or peer influencethat may
exert a stronger impact. Additionally, future studies could expand the scope by including larger and more
diverse samples across different grade levels, schools, or regions to enhance the generalizability of findings
and provide a more comprehensive understanding of the factors influencing students’ algebraic problem-
solving ability.
REFERENCES
1. Alvi, M. (2016). A manual for selecting sampling techniques in research. MPRA Paper No. 70218.
https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/70218/
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2513
www.rsisinternational.org
2. Anderson-Waugh, C., & George, L. (2024). Exploring mathematics anxiety and achievement: Insights
from a coeducational secondary school in the Turks and Caicos Islands. Caribbean Journal of
Education and Development, 1(2), 115. https://doi.org/10.46425/cjed101023567
3. Ashcraft, M. H., & Faust, M. W. (1994). Mathematics anxiety and mental arithmetic performance: An
exploratory investigation. Cognition and Emotion, 8(2), 97125.
4. Ashcraft, M. H., & Krause, J. A. (2007). Working memory, math performance, and math anxiety.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 243248.
5. Asomah, R. K., Magurd, J. K., Assamah, G., Narh-Kert, M., & Manu, H. N. (2025). The influence of
gender differences in mathematics anxiety on performance among senior high school students in
Ghana. International Journal of Education, Innovation, and Research, 4(1), 1633.
https://doi.org/10.31949/ijeir.v4i1.10565
6. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. W. H. Freeman and Company.
7. Baroody, A. J., Feil, Y., & Johnson, A. R. (2007). An alternative reconceptualization of procedural and
conceptual knowledge. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38(2), 115131.
8. Beilock, S. L., & Maloney, E. A. (2015). Math anxiety: A factor in math achievement not to be
ignored. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2(1), 412.
9. Camarista, G. G. (2015). Creativity, Self-Efficacy, Anxiety, and Problem-Solving Performance.
WVSU Research Journal, 4(2), 117.
10. Delastri, L., & Lolang, E. (2023). Students’ conceptual error and procedural error in solving algebraic
problems. Multicultural Education, 9(1), 1824. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7508092
11. Dowker, A., Sarkar, A., & Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics anxiety: What have we learned in 60
years? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 508.
12. El Houari, H., Khoule, A., & Osei Bonsu, N. (2020). Impact of conceptual and procedural knowledge
on students’ mathematics anxiety. Journal of Educational Psychology Research, 12(3), 134150.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/343019095
13. Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics anxiety. Journal for Research in
Mathematics Education, 21(1), 3346. https://doi.org/10.2307/749455
14. Kenney, S. (2024). Unveiling the errors learners make when solving word problems. SAGE Open.
https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/21582440241299245
15. Knuth, E. J., Stephens, A. C., McNeil, N. M., & Alibali, M. W. (2006). Does understanding the equal
sign matter? Evidence from solving equations. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 37(4),
297312.Perry, N. E., VandeKamp, K. O., Mercer, L. K., & Nordby, C. J. (2002). Investigating
teacherstudent interactions that foster self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 37(1), 515.
16. Mitchell, L., & George, L. (2022). Exploring mathematics anxiety among primary school students:
Prevalence, mathematics performance and gender. International Electronic Journal of Mathematics
Education, 17(3), Article em0692. https://doi.org/10.29333/iejme/12073
17. Polya, G. (1945). How to solve it: A new aspect of mathematical method. Princeton University Press.
18. Richardson, F. C., & Suinn, R. M. (1972). The Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale: Psychometric data.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 19(6), 551554.
19. Rittle-Johnson, B., & Schneider, M. (2015). Developing conceptual and procedural knowledge of
mathematics. In R. Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), Oxford handbook of numerical cognition (pp. 1102
1118). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199642342.013.014
20. Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory (Vol. 1). Springer.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8126-4
21. United Nations. (2015). Transforming our world: The 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development.
United Nations General Assembly. https://sdgs.un.org/2030agenda
22. Watson, R. (2015). Quantitative research. Nursing Standard, 29(31), 44.
https://doi.org/10.7748/ns.29.31.44.e8681
23. Xie, Y., Lan, X., & Tang, L. (2024). Gender differences in mathematics anxiety: A meta-analysis of
Chinese children. Acta Psychologica, 248, Article 104373.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2024.104373
24. Vicinanza, R. (2024). Secondary math teachers’ responses to errors in the classroom: Procedural,
conceptual, and factual errors (Undergraduate honors thesis, Pace University). Pace University
DigitalCommons. https://digitalcommons.pace.edu/honorscollege_theses/377
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF RESEARCH AND SCIENTIFIC INNOVATION (IJRSI)
ISSN No. 2321-2705 | DOI: 10.51244/IJRSI |Volume XII Issue VIII August 2025
Page 2514
www.rsisinternational.org
25. Young, C. B., Wu, S. S., & Menon, V. (2012). The neurodevelopmental basis of math anxiety.
Psychological Science, 23(5), 492501.
26. Yudhanegara, M. R., Sutiarso, S., & Saputri, D. (2023). Students’ conceptual error and procedural error
in solving algebraic problems. Journal of Mathematics Education Research, 14(1), 2334.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/367524159
27. Zhang, J., Zhao, N., & Kong, Q. P. (2019). The relationship between math anxiety and math
performance: A meta-analytic investigation. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1613.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01613